Future directions: what type of research do we need?

There is an urgent need for strategic research designed to generate and rigorously test candidate nongenetic, risk-modifying factors for schizophrenia. A framework for the primary prevention of schizophrenia can be built on this type of research.

While we have some broad clues in the form of risk indicators (urban birth, season of birth, pregnancy and birth complications), we need to ‘fine map’ these domains.

We should continue to try to identify novel domains from which to generate candidate exposures. One way to look for clues is to identify disease gradients and then search for clues to explain these gradients.  These gradients can be across time (season of birth, between-year variability) and across space (neighbourhood variation, urban - rural gradient and latitude gradients in prevalence). Within medical epidemiology, migrant studies have played an important role in generating candidate exposures. They can vary the environmental factors while ‘holding’ ethnicity constant (and,  it is assumed,  a degree of genetic variability)  (Hennekens and Buring, 1987). The cause of the increased risk of psychosis in second-generation African - Caribbeans born in the UK is still unclear. These studies of schizophrenia in migrant groups may help to generate awareness of novel candidate exposures (Harrison et al., 1997; McDonald and Murray, 2000).

It is essential that the data used to address this problem are derived from epidemiologically informed sampling frames. Sample sizes need to be representative of the underlying population so the PAR values that guide public health planning can be generated. Links between candidate exposures and genetic epidemiology will also be crucial for future research.

Just as candidate genes are ranked according to various rules (e.g.  are they located in a ‘hot spot’, how many introns/exons within the gene, biological plausibility, etc.?), those interested in exposures need sorting rules. When rank-ordering candidate exposures for more intense scrutiny, there is a case to prioritise those with universal public health potential (e.g. vaccinations for infectious agents, nutritional supplements). If the exposure is already associated with disorders other than schizophrenia, then this makes it a more attractive candidate from a public health perspective (single interventions could lead to improvements in multiple outcomes).

Epidemiological research can serve to ‘sharpen the focus’, allowing candidate risk factors to be identified. However, history has shown that risk factor epidemiology can sometimes enter cycles of uninformative replications (‘circular epidemiology’: Kuller, 1999). Season of birth has been studied as a risk factor for 80 years with little real progress in identifying the underlying risk-modifying exposure. In reaction to this, some research groups are looking to developments in the neurosciences in order to test the impact of candidate exposures on neuronal development, both in vitro and in whole animal studies. Such analyses range from gene expression profiling through to cognitive psychology techniques such as prepulse inhibition.

These techniques are removed from the schizophrenia phenotype, but nevertheless can provide valuable evidence about the biological plausibility of candidate risk factors. Dialogue between those involved in risk factor epidemiology and those in developmental neurobiology can provide new clues about the potential timing of exposures (e.g.  vulnerable periods of brain development)  and broad classes of exposure (e.g. exposures that may impact on neural apoptosis, migration, etc.).

Such scientific cross-fertilization can be informative for basic neuroscience as well.

Clues from epidemiology linked vitamin A and increased risk of craniofacial abnormality, which then led to the discovery of the role of retinoic acid in brain development and ultimately to its nomination as a potential risk factor for schizophrenia (LaMantia, 1999).

Animal models for schizophrenia based on developmental interventions include lesions in selected brain areas (Lipska et al., 1993) prenatal exposure to specific viruses such as influenza (Fatemi et al., 1999) and Borna virus (Hornig et al., 1999), and prenatal hypoxic/ischaemic insults (Mallard et al.,  1999).  The potential to extend this type of research with the use of genetic knock-out mice (to test gene - environment interactions) and gene expression profiling (cross-referencing altered gene expression in animal experiments with gene expression studies in schizophrenia) offers powerful new tools to the neuroscience community. This type of research may galvanize the search for novel nongenetic risk factors for schizophrenia.

Sartorius and Henderson (1992) proposed three options for the research community interested in the primary prevention of mental illness. The first option was to forget about primary prevention completely and concentrate on better treatments and cures. The second option was to fund more research in order to discover the causes of serious psychiatric illness and then commence primary prevention.


The final option was to start primary prevention based on existing knowledge, albeit imperfect. Clearly, we need a balance of all three approaches. Basic strategic research needs to go hand in hand with attempts at primary prevention. The nihilism and despair of the past needs to be replaced by a sense of determination and urgency. The dream of making the primary prevention of schizophrenia a reality may be quixotic, but it is certainly not impossible.

Acknowledgement

The project described in this section was supported by the Stanley Foundation.

John McGrath
Queensland Centre for Schizophrenia Research, Wolston Park Hospital, Wacol, Australia

REFERENCES

  • Barker DJP (1992) Fetal and Infant Origins of Adult Disease. London: British Medical Journal.
  • Bebbington P, Wilkins S, Jones P et al. (1993) Life events and psychosis: initital results from the Camberwell Collaborative Psychosis Study. British Journal of Psychiatry 162, 72 - 79.
  • Bennedsen BE (1998) Adverse pregnancy outcome in schizophrenic women: occurence and risk factors. Schizophrenia Research 33, 1 - 26.
  • Brown AS, Susser ES, Butler PD, Richardson AR, Kaufmann CA, Gorman JM (1996) Neurobiological plausibility of prenatal nutritional deprivation as a risk factor for schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 184, 71 - 85.
  • Fatemi SH, Emamian ES, Kist D et al. (1999) Defective corticogenesis and reduction in Reelin immunoreactivity in cortex and hippocampus of prenatally infected neonatal mice. Molecular Psychiatry 4, 145 - 154.
  • Gordon R (1983) An operational classification of disease prevention. Public Health Reports 98, 271 - 282.
  • Harrison G, Glazebrook C, Brewin J et al. (1997) Increased incidence of psychotic disorders in migrants from the Caribbean to the United Kingdom. Psychological Medicine 27, 799 - 806.
  • Hennekens CH, Buring JE (1987) Epidemiology in Medicine. Boston, MA: Little, Brown.
  • Hornig M, Weissenbock H, Horscroft N, Lipkin WI (1999) An infection-based model of neurodevelopmental damage. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 96, 12102 - 12107.
  • Kraemer HC, Kazdin AE, Offord DR, Kkessler RC, Jensen PS, Kupfer DJ (1997) Coming to terms with the terms of risk. Archives of General Psychiatry 54, 337 - 343.
  • Kuller LH (1999) Circular epidemiology [see comments]. American Journal of Epidemiology 150, 897 - 903.
  • LaMantia A (1999) Forebrain induction, retinoic acid, and vulnerability to schizophrenia: insights from molecular and genetic analysis in developing mice. Biological Psychiatry 46, 19 - 30.
  • Last JM (1988) A Dictionary of Epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Lipska BK, Jaskiw GE, Weinberger DR (1993) Postpubertal emergence of hyperresponsiveness to stress and to amphetamine after neonatal excitotoxic hippocampal damage: a potential animal model of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 9, 67 - 75.
  • Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ (1998) Randomised trial of early diet in preterm babies and later intelligence quotient. British Medical Journal 317, 1481 - 1487.
  • Mallard EC, Rehn A, Rees S, Tolcos M, Copolov D (1999) Ventriculomegaly and reduced hippocampal volume following intrauterine growth-restriction: implications for the aetiology of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 40, 11 - 21.
  • Marcelis M, Navarro-Mateu F, Murray R, Selten J-P, van Os J (1998) Urbanization and psychosis:a study of 1942 - 1978 birth cohorts in the Netherlands. Psychological Medicine 28, 871 - 879.
  • McDonald C, Murray RM (2000) Early and late environmental risk factors for schizophrenia. Brain Research Reviews 31, 130 - 137.
  • McGorry PD, Jackson HJ (1999) The Recognition and Management of Early Psychosis: a Preventive Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • McGrath JJ (1999) Hypothesis: is low prenatal vitamin D a risk-modifying factor for schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Research 40, 173 - 177.
  • McGrath J, Castle D (1995) Does influenza cause schizophrenia? A five year review. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 29, 23 - 31.
  • McGrath J, McGlashan TH (1999) Improving outcomes for recent-onset psychoses: disentangling hope, speculation and evidence. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 100, 83 - 84.
  • McGrath JJ, Welham JL (1999) Season of birth and schizophrenia: a systematic review and metaanalysis of data from the Southern Hemisphere. Schizophrenia Research 35, 237 - 242.
  • Mednick SA, Schulsinger F, Venables PH (1981) The Mauritius Project. In: Prospective
  • Longitudinal Research: An Empirical Basis for the Primary Prevention of Psychosocial Disorders, Mednick SA, Baert A, eds. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 314 - 316.
  • Mednick SA, Parnas J, Schulsinger F (1987) The Copenhagen High-Risk Project, 1962 - 86. Schizophrenia Bulletin 13, 485 - 495.
  • Mortensen PB (2000) Urban - rural differences in the risk for schizophrenia. International Journal of Mental Health 29, 101 - 110.
  • Mortensen PB, Pedersen CB, Westergaard T et al. (1999) Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia. New England Journal of Medicine 340, 603 - 608.
  • Mrazek PJ, Haggerty RJ (1994) Reducing Risk for Mental Disorders: Frontiers for Preventive Intervention Research. Washington, DC: National Academic Press.
  • Murray CJ, Lopez AD (1996) The Global Burden of Disease. Boston, MA: Harvard School of Public Health.
  • Raine A, Venables PH, Mednick SA (1997) Low resting heart rate at age 3 years predisposes to aggression at age 11 years: evidence from the Mauritius Child Health Project. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 36, 1457 - 1464.
  • Rantakallio P, Jones P, Moring J, von Wendt, L (1997) Associations between central nervous system infections during childhood and adult onset schizophrenia and other psychoses: A 28- year follow-up. International Review of Epidemiology 26, 837 - 843.
  • Rose G (1992) The Strategy of Preventive Medicine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Sacker A, Done DJ, Crow TJ (1996) Obstetric complications in children born to parents with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of case-control studies. Psychological Medicine 26, 279 - 287.
  • Salvatore M, Morzunov S, Schwemmle M, Lipkin WI (1997) Borna disease virus in brains of North American and European people with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Bornavirus Study Group. Lancet 349, 1813 - 1814.
  • Sartorius N, Henderson AS (1992) The neglect of prevention in psychiatry. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 26, 550 - 553.
  • Schulsinger F, Parnas J, Mednick S, Teasdale TW, Schulsinger H (1987) Heredity - environment interaction and schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research 21, 431 - 436.
  • Scott JM, Weir DG, Molloy A, McPartlin J, Daly L, Kirke P (1994) Folic Acid Metabolism and Mechanisms of Neural Tube Defect. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
  • Susser E, Neugebauer R, Hoek H et al. (1996) Schizophrenia after prenatal famine: further evidence. Archives of General Psychiatry 53, 25 - 31.
  • Susser EB, Brown A, Matte TD (1999) Prenatal factors and adult mental and physical health. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 44, 326 - 334.
  • Susser M (1991) What is a cause and how do we know one? A grammar for pragmatic epidemiology. American Journal of Epidemiology 133, 635 - 648.
  • Tarrant CJ, Jones PB (1999) Precursors to schizophrenia: do biological markers have specificity? Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 44, 335 - 349.
  • Tienari P, Wynne LC, Moring J et al. (1994) The Finnish Adoptive Family Study of schizophrenia. Implications for family research. British Journal of Psychiatry Suppl. 20 - 26.
  • Torrey EF, Miller J, Rawlings R, Yolken RH (1997) Seasonality of births in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a review of the literature. Schizophrenia Research 28, 1 - 38.
  • Tsuang MT, Stone WS, Faraone SV (2000) Towards the prevention of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 48, 349 - 356.
  • van Os J, Fahy TA, Bebbington P et al. (1994) The influence of life events on the subsequent course of psychotic illness. A prospective follow-up of the Camberwell Collaborative Psychosis Study. Psychological Medicine 24, 503 - 513.
  • Wahlbeck K, Forsen T, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Eriksson JG (2001) Association of schizophrenia with low maternal body mass index, small size at birth, and thinness during childhood. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 48 - 52.
  • Welham J, McLachlan G, Davies G, McGrath J (2000) Heterogeneity in schizophrenia; mixture modelling of age-at-first-admission, gender and diagnosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 101, 312 - 317.

Provided by ArmMed Media